Tuesday, December 27, 2016

Remembering the "individual" in individual differences research: A quote to note

I just ran across this statement in a recent article (see below). It served as a reminder of something I have always preached, but from-time-to-time, tend to forget as I analyze cognitive ability test data, post research articles, or suggest hypotheses regarding test score differences---be it here at this blog, in a journal article, book, book chapter, or professional presentation. The point being that we must remain vigilant in remembering the "individual" in individual differences research.

The privileged unit of analysis in psychology is the individual (Nesselroade, Gerstorf, Hardy, & Ram, 2007). Nevertheless, many data-analytic approaches coarsely aggregate data and tacitly assume group-average models to hold and to be interpreted in lieu of more fine-grained and, ultimately, person-specific models. For example, when a group of persons show an average increase of performance in a learning task, this does not mean that all persons follow a pattern of change similar to this average. In fact, none of the persons may be well represented by the average trend. In a similar vein, Tucker (1966) argued that the consideration of differences instead of averages will allow us to gain more information about the nature of basic functions underlying behavior. Ever since, researchers have been questioning coarse aggregation of data across persons (e.g., Lamiell, 1981; Nesselroade & Molenaar, 1999) as the estimates of averaged effects may not be representative of any single individual. In fact, strong inference about intra-individual variation from interindividual variation is only possible under the ergodic assumption (Molenaar, 2004), which assumes that the group model represents each individual's dynamics (homogeneity) and that those dynamics have constant characteristics in time (stationarity). In the same vein, Simpson (1951) pointed out that a statistical relationship observed in a population could be reversed within subgroups that form the population. For instance, “It may be universally true that drinking coffee increases one's level of neuroticism; then it may still be the case that people who drink more coffee are less neurotic” (Borsboom, Kievit, Cervone, & Hood, 2009, p. 72). Simpson's paradox may arise whenever inferences are drawn across different explanatory levels, for example, from populations to the individual, or from cross-sectional data to intraindividual change over time (see Kievit, Frankenhuis, Waldorp, & Borsboom, 2013, for further illustrations). Hence, there still is a need for focusing on individuals or subgroups of
individuals to more accurately model individual process idiosyncrasies and similarities across persons. Particularly, in light of large-scale empirical data sets, aggregation is more likely to lead to models with low informative value about individual underlying processes as it is often difficult to expand prior hypotheses to account for the large number of potential explanatory variables.


Quote is from this article:




- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

What do IQ researchers really think about the Flynn Effect?

Read on Flipboard
Read on unz.com



This email was sent to you by a Flipboard user via Flipboard's "Send to Friends" feature. Need help? Email support@flipboard.com.

Privacy Policy    Unsubscribe

©2016 Flipboard, Inc. Made with love at 735 Emerson Street, Palo Alto, CA 94301

- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
 

Monday, December 26, 2016

Research Bytes: The Flynn effect in the Czech Republic via BrowZine

The Flynn effect in the Czech Republic
Laciga, Jiří; Cígler, Hynek
Intelligence: Articles in press



University of Minnesota Users:
http://login.ezproxy.lib.umn.edu/login?url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289616302239

Non-University of Minnesota Users: (Full text may not be available)
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289616302239

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.

Saturday, December 24, 2016

Research Bytes: Overlap Between the General Factor of Personality and Emotional Intelligence: A Meta-Analysis. via BrowZine

Overlap Between the General Factor of Personality and Emotional Intelligence: A Meta-Analysis.
van der Linden, Dimitri; Pekaar, Keri A.; Bakker, Arnold B.; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Vernon, Philip A.; Dunkel, Curtis S.; Petrides, K. V.
Psychological Bulletin: Articles in press



University of Minnesota Users:
http://login.ezproxy.lib.umn.edu/login?url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/bul0000078

Non-University of Minnesota Users: (Full text may not be available)
http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/bul0000078

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.

Wednesday, December 7, 2016

Miranda at 50: A Psychological Analysis via BrowZine

Miranda at 50: A Psychological Analysis
Smalarz, L.; Scherr, K. C.; Kassin, S. M.
Current Directions in Psychological Science: Vol. 25 Issue 6 – 2016: 455 - 460

10.1177/0963721416665097

University of Minnesota Users:
http://login.ezproxy.lib.umn.edu/login?url=http://cdp.sagepub.com/lookup/doi/10.1177/0963721416665097

Non-University of Minnesota Users: (Full text may not be available)
http://cdp.sagepub.com/lookup/doi/10.1177/0963721416665097

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.

Saturday, December 3, 2016

Research Bytes: What is the Flynn Effect, and how does it change our understanding of IQ? via BrowZine

What is the Flynn Effect, and how does it change our understanding of IQ?
Shenk, David
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science: Articles in press



University of Minnesota Users:
http://login.ezproxy.lib.umn.edu/login?url=http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.1366/pdf

Non-University of Minnesota Users: (Full text may not be available)
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/wcs.1366/pdf

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.